Construction grammar
Homework
She sneezed the napkin off the table
The issue with this sentence is that sneeze is typically an intransitive verb which does not take an object. Here it takes an object (the napkin), and a Prepositional Phrase expressing location (off the table). This is a very unusual use of sneeze. However, the meaning is transparent, that our sneeze propells the napkin off the table. The question is how are we able to comprehenend and produce such an utterance?
Traditional verb-centric story: we access the verb, then retrieve the argument structure from the lemma.
PROBLEM: sneeze is unlikely to specify an argument structure with 3 entities. It is theoretically possible that we have a special meaning of sneeze in our lexicons, e.g. sneeze = make a sudden involuntary expulsion of air from the nose and mouth due to irritation of one's nostrils such that the resulting air flow propels an object X from location Y. But this is unlikely given the rarity of sentences with sneeze and three arguments.
The meaning of the sentence comes, in part, form the sentence frame or 'construction'.
The construction NP$_1$ + V + NP$_2$ + PP means NP$_1$ acts upon NP$_2$ such that NP$_2$ changes location. The verb is then 'squished' into the construction, and the two meanings (verb + construction) are 'fused'
By assuming that constructions have meaning, we can overcome a lot of problems with traditional Verb Argument Structure accounts.
Problems with traditional Verb Argument Structure accounts
1. Valency augmentation
We often use sentences with more arguments than the standard valency, e.g.
She sneezed the napkin off the table (example above)
The naughty students laughed the teacher out of the room
She juggled her way to the semi-finals of the juggling contest
Can you chop me some carrots?
Quote me happy! (Recent advert for insurance)
2. Weird thematic relations
There are plenty of verbs which take arguments with 'weird' roles, e.e.g
She is happy
The table needs a leg
That coat suits you
The coat fits me
She swapped her Porsche for a Ferrari
Both a Porsche and a Ferrari can be described as PATIENTS, but this undermines the principle that there is differentiation between the arguments
They loaded the truck with hay
Is truck a LOCATIVE or a PATIENT? If it is a PATIENT what kind of role does hay play? (assuming that all roles need to be differentiated?)
None of these roles can be easily described using standard labels. There is no list of thematic relations which can account for all arguments in English, or indeed any language. This is despite many dozens of attempts by linguists.
How constructions solve these issues
Goldberg's theory of Verb Argument Structure Constructions
Goldberg (1995) proposes that when we use verbs in setnences, we insert them into Verb Argument Structure Constructions (VAS Constructions). These are syntactic units with their own independent meaning. This can help explain both valency augmentation and weird thematic relations:
Valency augmentation
Here are the two possible solutions:
The multiple sense account. We have different senses of verbs in our lexicons, some of which allow extra arguments. e.g. there is a lexical entry for sneeze which allows for three arguments
The construction based account. The extra argument is specified by the construction itself.
The multiple sense account is highly implausible. Given how rare it is to use sneeze with three arguments, it is very unlikely that we have such a representation in the lexicon.
Weird thematic relations
If roles are (in part) given by the construction itself, we are not committed to a small and finite set of roles. For example we could posit the following constructions:
The copula construction:
FORM: X is Y
FUNCTION 1: X denotes the same referent as Y (e.g. One man's terrorist is another man's Freedom Fighter)
FUNCTION 2: Y describes a property of X (e.g. Die hard is a great Xmas film)
FUNCTION 3 (where X and Y are identical): X/Y has a property which is relevant to the current discussion (e.g. It wasn't a great film, but hey, George Clooney is George Clooney)
The "needing" construction:
FORM: X + needing verb + Y
FUNCTION: X needs Y (e.g. we're going to need a bigger boat, this task requires a bigger boat, this task necessitates a bigger boat)
The fit/suit construction:
FORM: X + fit/suit + Y.
FUNCTION: X (an item of clothing) looks good on Y (a person)
How constructions combine with verbs
Fusion
But surely the verbs must provide a role as well? Yes, verbs specify roles which are highly specific to that verb, while the VAS Construction specifies roles which are more general. When using a verb with a particular VAS Construction, we need to "fuse" the verb-specific role in the former, with the more general role in the latter.
What about traditional roles, e.g. "AGENT" / "PATIENT"?
Many of the roles specified by VAS Constructions correspond to widely used traditional labels such as "AGENT" and "PATIENT", but many do not (e.g. the subject of the copula, or the subject of need).
Some roles appear to be more general than others because they
Occur in VAS Constructions with very general/abstract meanings and which allow lots of different verbs, e.g. AGENT + VERB + PATIENT
Describe slots with similar meanings in related constructions, e.g.
THEME + VERB + EXP. = She scares me, They please George, The play entertained me
EXP. + VERB + THEME = I fear her, George likes them, I enjoyed the play
The implications of VAS constructions
The idea of VAS constructions contradicts with traditional "words and rules" approaches to grammar. According to these approaches, words are combined using rules, and then are semantically interpreted, e.g.
RULE: VP <- V + NP
RULE: Sentence <- NP + VP
INTERPRETATION: A situation involving two NPs whose roles are determined by the verb.
Late semantic interpretation can be explained by the fact that we always try to identify a meaning as long as the sentence is well formed, e.g.
Colourless green ideas sleep furiously
Words and rules approaches draw a categorical distinction between syntax (combinatorial rules) and the lexicon (repository of meaning)
However, according to Goldberg, syntactic constructions can also be meaningful. This breaks down the distinction between syntax and the lexicon. According to Goldberg, they are all part of the same system. Words involve mapping between form and meaning. Constructions also involve a mapping between form and meaning.
Other types of construction (not VAS)
Multi-word constructions are everywhere, e.g.
You mean, I've got Arthritis Arthritis!!??
FORM: The noun is repeated.
MEANING: There is a scale of authenticity ranging from N to N + N (Arthritis to Arthritis Arthritis). N + N (Arthritis Arthritis) is at the high end of this scale.
There are burgers, and then there are burgers
FORM: There is/are + NP and then there is/are + NP
MEANING: There is a scale of authenticity, with the second NP at the top of the scale.
Now watch me go and drop this cake!
FORM: Now watch + Clause (with nonfinite verb)
MEANING: Sometimes called the "tempting fate" construction. It means I hope X DOESN'T happen, e.g. I hope I DON'T drop this cake.
She worked her way to the top of the class
FORM: Subject + Verb + Possessive determiner (his, her, its) + "way" + PREPOSITIONAL PHRASE
MEANING: X arrived at location Z by means of action Y. Progress is difficult, and the path is circuitous
These all display correspondences between form and meaning. The meanings are not predictable from general linguistic rules, i.e. they are non-compositional.
Comparing the two models
According to the Words and Rules Approach
There is a categorical distinction between words (meaning) and rules (form)
Linguistic knowledge consists of (a) lexical knowledge, and (b) syntactic knowledge (ability to combine words)
Semantic interpretation of a sentence is seperate (and subsequent to) syntactic interpretation
According to Construction Grammar accounts
Words and syntactic constructions are all part of the same system, sometimes called the "Constructicon"
Linguistic knowledge = knowledge of constructions (words can be described as "minimal constructions")
Semantics and syntax are inseparable (as syntactic constructions are meaningful)
Problems for construction grammar
Some syntactic phenomena do not appear to be linked to meaning, e.g. subject + verb agreement.
Clinical implications
When teaching children grammatical constructions we need to higlight the meaning of those constructions. E.g. Riches (2013) highlights the meaning of the passive construction (the verb is highly affected), e.g.
The vase was smashed by the dog (Vase is highly affected)
This is reflected in children's early use of the passive. We very rarely have passives in which the subject is not highly affected, e.g.
It was dreamt about.
Bibliography
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: a Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. University of Chicago Press.
Riches, N. G. (2013). Treating the passive in children with specific language impairment: A usage-based approach. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 29(2), 155–169.
Last updated